After every project presented below, I list the most important publications reflecting its results, my collaborators and students who contributed to it. Wherever earlier publications, e.g. in conference proceedings, were superseded by subsequent journal articles, only the latter are shown. Here is a longer list of selected publications with download links. If the findings have not been written up yet, they are reflected in my conference presentations.
1. Grammatical gender and case. Agreement in number, case, gender and person
Taken together, this is my biggest current project, with many research lines, dozens of papers and conference presentations.
1a. Agreement. This project is dedicated to the study of agreement in number, gender, case and — to a lesser extent — person. Agreement is one of the most basic mechanisms in the grammar, and the project also sheds new light on the nature of features (for example, on the problem of feature markedness), as well as on some other topics, like the role of morphological ambiguity, or syncretism, in the mental grammar. We also focused on different properties of inflections (for example, how frequently a particular inflection is used to encode a particular feature, how 'typical' it is for a given gender or case) and analyzed how this affects processing. We mostly used behavioral experiments, but also conducted one EEG and one fMRI study.
The phenomenon of agreement attraction (specific agreement errors that are often produced and easily missed by native speakers, which gives us a window into the mechanisms of agreement) drew attention of researchers around the world. Many experiments we did focus on attraction. We were the first to study gender agreement attraction in comprehension, the role of systematic and accidental syncretism in production and processing of number agreement attraction and in processing of gender agreement attraction, as well as the processes similar (but not identical) to attraction in case agreement. I also took part in the study of person agreement attraction led by Anna Laurinavichyute.
In addition to that, we compared processing of different features. We studied processing of case, gender and number agreement in participial clauses and in predicative agreement, both with behavioural methods and using ERPs. Interesting differences between these features and their values have been revealed.
Finally, through this project, I got interested in gender and case features in a more general context.
1b. Gender. We analyze how different semantic and formal properties of the noun interact with its gender features in the mental grammar. We look at expressive nouns (with diminutive and augmentative affixes), nouns denoting professions (feminitives, both established and newly formed, as well as historically masculine nouns that now can be used as common gender nouns with feminine agreement — but not all of them and not in all syntactic contexts), at nouns with different stem-final consonants and affixes, at indeclinable nouns, at recent borrowings etc. We use production and comprehension experiments and different corpora. Varvara Magomedova played the leading role in many studies on gender, and Kirill Chuprinko is the main contributor in the study of indeclinable nouns. As a result, we came up with a new approach to the problem of gender markedness in Russian.
1c. Case. We have a big project dedicated to the acquisition of case system and case processing in Russian as a foreign language (see project 4) and conducted several experiments examining case processing by native speakers. Most previous studies, both in Russian and in other languages, looked at different case forms presented in isolation, while we analyze them in a sentential context.
Collaborators: Varvara Magomedova, Anton Malko, Natalia Cherepovskaia, Daria Chernova, Svetlana Alexeeva, Krill Bursov, Kirill Chuprinko, Daria Antropova, Anna Laurinavichyute. Students: Yulia Vakulenko, Ekaterina Nikulina, Anna Stetsenko, Tatiana Matyushkina, Pavel Shilin, Alexander Nozdrin, Natalia Chuprasova, Anastasia Generalova, Maria Chebkasova, Maya Korotkaya, Regina Starovoytova, Anton Guldinsky, Daria Alfimova, Elizaveta Vilenchik, Marina Frolova, Daria Petrova, Polina Makarova, Polina Leonova, Anastasia Ivanova, Polina Skripkina.
Chuprinko, K., Magomedova, V., & Slioussar, N. (2024). Modeling gender variation in Russian indeclinable nouns: Optimality over structuralism, hierarchical MaxEnt, and degrees of idiosyncrasy. Accepted to the Journal of Slavic Linguistics. Download. — On gender of indeclinable Russian nouns (comparing different frameworks to opt for an optimality-theoretic approach and to build a hierarchical MaxEnt model).
Chuprinko, K., Magomedova, V., & Slioussar, N. (2023). Gender variation in indeclinable inanimate nouns and gender markedness in modern Russian. Acta Linguistica Academica, 70, 317–338. Doi: 10.1556/2062.2023.00641. Download. — On gender of indeclinable Russian nouns (a corpus study, a description of variation, implications for the gender markedness problem).
Magomedova, V., & Slioussar, N. (2023). Gender variation and gender markedness in Russian nouns. Voprosy Jazykoznanija, 2, 7-28. Doi 10.31857/0373-658X.2023.2.7-28. Download. — A new approach to gender markedness in Russian, data on expressive nouns and on gender assignment depending on the final consonant of the stem.
An overview of many things we did on gender can be found in the summary of Varvara Magomedova's PhD thesis.
Slioussar, N. (2023). The role of case syncretism in sentence processing. In: P. Caha, P. Rudnev, & S. Toldova (eds.), The many facets of agreement (pp. 145-163). München: Lincom. Download. — An overview of our experiments focusing on the role of syncretism in processing.
Chernova, D., Slioussar, N., Alexeeva, S., & Bursov, K. (2022). Trans-paradigmatic syncretism in case form processing in Russian. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 30, 1-16. Available at: http://ojs.ung.si/index.php/JSL/article/view/97. Download. — Most previous studies, both in Russian and in other languages, looked at different case forms presented in isolation, while we analyze them in a sentential context. We show that the factors that played a role in isolation become irrelevant and discover an unexpected facilitarory effect of trans-paradigmatic syncretism.
Slioussar, N., Magomedova, V., & Makarova, P. (2022). The role of case syncretism in agreement attraction: a comprehension study. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 829112. Doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.829112. Download. — The first study targeting the role of head and dependent noun syncretism in gender agreement attraction processing.
A presentation from the AMLaP 2022 talk (with Daria Antropova and Daria Chernova) describing differences in gender, number and case processing in participial clauses modifying complex NPs.
Magomedova, V., & Slioussar, N. (2021). Gender and case in Russian nouns denoting professions and social roles. Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 20, 483-491. Doi: 10.28995/2075-7182-2021-20-483-491. Download. — Nouns denoting professions and social roles that used to be masculine can now be used with feminine agreement, but are normative only in the nominative. So extensive previous research focused on nominative forms, while we analysed oblique cases for the first time. This paper contains corpus data showing that feminine agreement is used in oblique cases, although much less frequently, and three experiments exploring processing differences between oblique cases. We attribute some interesting differences we discovered to trans-paradigmatic syncretism.
Frolova, M., & Slioussar, N. (2021). Professions and gender agreement in Russian. In: A. Botinis (ed.), ExLing 2021: Proceedings of 12th International Conference of Experimental Linguistics (pp. 89-92). Athens: ExLing Society. Doi:10.36505/ExLing-2021/12/0023/000496. Download. — Most nouns denoting professions are historically masculine, but can now be also used with feminine agreement (at least, in nominative). At the same time, some of these nouns have paired feminine nouns. We investigated for the first time how the availability of such pairs influences the processing of these nouns with feminine agreement.
Slioussar, N. (2018). Forms and features: the role of syncretism in number agreement attraction. Journal of Memory and Language, 101, 51–63. Doi 10.1016/j.jml.2018.03.006. Download. — The first study targeting the role of systematic and accidental syncretism in number agreement attraction in production and processing.
Slioussar, N. (2018). Gender, declension and stem-final consonants: An experimental study of gender agreement in Russian. Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 17, 688-700. Download. — Studying gender agreement attraction processing depending on the '(proto)typicality' of inflections.
Slioussar, N., Stetsenko, A., & Matyushkina, T. (2017). Producing case errors in Russian. In: Y. Oseki et al. (eds.). Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics: The New York Meeting 2015 (pp. 363-379). Ann Arbor, MI: Michigan Slavic Publications. Download. — Studying an effect similar to agreement attraction in case errors in production.
Slioussar, N., & Malko, A. (2016). Gender agreement attraction in Russian: production and comprehension evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1651. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01651. Download. — One of the first studies of gender agreement attraction in comprehension, focusing on the problem of feature markedness in a languague with three genders.
Chernova, D., Slioussar, N., Prokopenya, V., Petrova, T., & Chernigovskaya, T. (2016). Eksperimental'nye issledovanija grammatiki: sintaksicheskij analiz neodnoznachnyx predlozhѕenij (in Russian, ‘Experimental studies of the grammar: syntactic analysis of ambiguous sentences’). Voprosy Jazykoznanija, 6, 36-50. Download. — Studying differences in number and case processing in participial clauses modifying complex NPs (this project was continued by Antropova, Chernova and Slioussar and presented in their 2022 AMLaP talk).
Slioussar, N., & Cherepovskaia, N. (2014). Case errors in processing: Evidence from Russian. In: C. Chapman, O. Kit, & I. KuДЌerova (eds.). Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics: The First Hamilton Meeting 2013 (pp. 319-338). Ann Arbor, MI: Michigan Slavic Publications. Download. — Studying an effect similar to agreement attraction in case errors in comprehension.
2. Inflectional morphology in the brain
How we produce and process different word forms is one of the main topics in experimental linguistics. In this project, we used neuroimaging methods (fMRI and EEG) to find out whether these is a principled difference between regular and irregular forms, and, if yes, what the nature of regularity is: is it associated with inflectional class (type) frequency, productivity or defaultness? Almost all previous fMRI and EEG studies focusing on morphological regularity were conducted on languages with relatively poor morphology, so Russian allows addressing this problem on a new level, teasing apart the role of different factors. Recently, we conducted an EEG experiment and observed a difference between regular and irregular verbs that is very similar to the AoA (age of acquisition) effect — in the previous studies, such effects were found for words with a different AoA, but not for inflectional classes.
Collaborators: Maxim Kireev and his colleagues from the N.P. Bekhtereva Institute of the Human Brain, Russian Academy of Sciences.
Slioussar, N., Korotkov, A., Cherednichenko, D., Chernigovskaya, T., & Kireev, M. (2024). Exploring the nature of morphological regularity: an fMRI study on Russian. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 39, 24-39. Online first in 2023. Doi: 10.1080/23273798.2023.2237138. Download.
Kireev, M.V., Slioussar, N.A., Korotkov, A.D., Kotomin, I.A., Masharipov, R.S., Chernigovskaya, T.V., & Medvedev, S.V. (2020). Reorganization of functional interactions in the frontotemporal system of the human brain during production of Russian verbs. Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 50, 349-357. Doi: 10.1007/s11055-020-00907-0. Download.
Kireev, M.V., Slioussar, N., Chernigovskaya, T.V., Korotkov, A.D., & Medvedev, S.V. (2015). Changes in functional connectivity within the fronto-temporal brain network induced by regular and irregular Russian verb production. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 36. Download.
Slioussar, N., Kireev, M.V., Chernigovskaya, T.V., Kataeva, G.V., Korotkov, A.D., & Medvedev, S.V. (2014). An ER-fMRI study of Russian inflectional morphology. Brain and Language, 130, 33-41. Download.
3. Information structure (IS), word order and prosody in Russian
After a four-year break, I came back to the topics I studied before 2014 — but this time together with the students from the Laboratory of Formal Methods in Linguistics and some other coauthors. We used corpus and experimental methods to study SOV and VSO word orders, constructions with fronted foci, double object constructions, and the role of syntactic and prosodic means used to encode IS in production and in comprehension. We also compared word order processing in native and L2 speakers of Russian (see project 4). The last question was also explored for Adyghe (also known as West Circassian), a language from the Northwest Caucasian family spoken in the Republic of Adygea in Russia. We also did some corpus and experimental work on binding (personal pronouns, anaphors and reciprocals) depending on the predicate type and word order. Below, I list both older and more recent papers (unfortunately, most of the recent work has not been published yet).
Collaborators: Svetlana Toldova. Students: Natalia Tyshkevich, Sofiya Popova, Ilya Makarchuk, Denis Rahman, Anastasia Sidorova, Maxim Bazhukov, Lyubov Chubarova, Anton Buzanov, Zinaida Budilova.
Slioussar, N., & Rakhman, D. (2024). Narrative inversion in Russian. Submitted ms. Download.
Slioussar, N., & Makarchuk, I. (2023). SOV in Russian: a corpus study. Journal of Slavic Linguistics, 30, 1-14. Download.
Buzanov, A., Toldova, S., Budilova, Z., & Slioussar, N. (2022). Non-canonical constructions with reflexive possessives in Russian: u-possessor constructions. Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 21 (2), 1029-1042. Doi: 10.28995/2075-7182-2022-21-1029-1042. Download.
Chubarova, L., Bazhukov, M., Slioussar, N., & Toldova, S. (2022). Otnositel'nyj porjadok dativnogo argumenta v ditranzitivnyx konstrukcijax v russkom jazyke: korpusnye i eksperimental'nye issledovanija (in Russian, ‘Relative order of dative arguments in ditransitive constructions in Russian: Corpus and experimental studies’). Typology of Morphosyntactic Parameters, 5 (2), 101–123. Download.
Bazhukov, M., Chubarova, L, Slioussar, N., & Toldova, S. (2021). The order of objects in Russian: A corpus study. Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 20, 68-78. Doi: 10.28995/2075-7182-2021-20-68-78. Download.
Slioussar, N. (2015). Interpreting sentences without prosody and context. In: P. Arkadiev et al. (eds.). Donum semanticum: Opera linguistica et logica in honorem Barbarae Partee a discipulis amicisque Rossicis oblate (pp. 285-296). Moscow: Languages of Slavic Culture. Download.
Slioussar, N. (2014). What is and what is not problematic about the T-model. In: P. Kosta et al. (eds.). Minimalism and Beyond: Radicalizing the Interfaces (pp. 350-362). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. Download.
Slioussar, N. (2011). Processing of a free word order language: The role of syntax and context. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 40, 291–306. Download.
Slioussar, N. (2011). Russian and the EPP requirement in the Tense domain. Lingua, 121, 2048-2068. Download.
Slioussar, N. (2010). Russian data call for relational Information Structure notions. In: Zybatow G. et al. (eds.). Formal Studies in Slavic Linguistics. Proceedings of Formal Description of Slavic Languages 7.5 (pp. 329-344). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang. Download.
4. Inflectional morphology (primarily case) and word order in Russian as a foreign language: acquisition and processing
On case. It has been extensively studied how Russian children acquire the case system, but we know much less about mastering this crucial piece of Russian grammar when learning Russian as a foreign, or second, language (L2). Firstly, using written texts elicited from Catalan students with different proficiency levels, we established the order in which cases are acquired (nominative, locative, accusative, genitive, instrumental, dative), as well as certain characteristics of their acquisition trajectories. We argue that the order of acquisition is determined by two groups of factors: how essential a given case is for successful language use and how complex it is, both semantically and morphologically. The latter is irrelevant for the L1 acquisition. Many other important differences between the first and second language acquisition were identified. We show that the maturation of the case system can be observed both in the number of correct case forms participants produced and in error rates — this is important because many L2 studies tend to focus on errors ignoring the correct forms.
Secondly, we conducted experiments targeting different aspects of L2 case processing. Native speakers of Catalan, Spanish and English with different levels of L2 Russian took part in them. As a result, we could contribute to the main debate in the field of L2 processing: how and why it is different from L1 processing and which mechanisms they share.
Now we are extending this project to study L2 acquisition and processing of other morphological features in Russian and to involve participants with other L1s to assess the role of the native language.
On word order. We also studied how native speakers of Russian and speakers of Mandarin Chinese learning Russian process Russian sentences with different word orders. We compared SVO and OVS orders in isolation and in different contexts. Experiments focusing on the L2 processing of different word orders are still not very numerous, and those using context are extremely rare. Non-canonical orders were difficult for L2, but not for L1 participants. However, L1 participants gave non-canonical orders lower ratings in isolation, presumably because in naturally occurring texts, they are used only in particular contexts. As for the context factor, some effects were the same for L1 and L2 processing. However, unlike native speakers, L2 readers are not sensitive to more subtle contextual requirements of different word orders.
Collaborators: Natalia Cherepovskaia, Anna Denissenko, Maria Grabovskaya, Anastasia Ivanenko. Students: Elizaveta Reutova, Maria Harchevnik.
Cherepovskaia, N., & Slioussar, N. (2024). Processing case errors in L1 and L2 Russian. Submitted ms. Download.
Slioussar, N., & Harchevnik, M. (2024). Word order and context in sentence processing: evidence from L1 and L2 Russian. Frontiers in Psychology, 15, 1344366. Doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1344366. Download.
Cherepovskaia, N., Slioussar, N., & Denissenko, A. (2022). Acquisition of the nominal case system in Russian as a second language. Second Language Research, 38, 555-580. Doi: 10.1177/0267658320988058. Download.
Cherepovskaia, N., Reutova, E., & Slioussar, N. (2021). Becoming native-like for good or ill: Online and offline processing of case forms in l2 Russian. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 652463. Doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.652463. Download.
5. Reading in L1 vs. in L2
Even when we know a foreign language really well, we feel that reading in the native language is different. But what exactly is different and which factors influence these differences? We take part in several international and local projects trying to answer these questions.
Reading relies on different competences: orthographic and phonological competence, vocabulary size etc. Working memory and general intelligence may also play a role. To assess how these competences contribute to L1 and L2 reading and how your L1 and your L1 reading habits influence your reading in L2, the Multilingual Eye-tracking Corpus (MECO) has been created.
The MECO project is coordinated by Viktor Kuperman and his team at MacMaster University, Canada. Initially, 12 labs participated, but the number is much bigger now. Every lab collected data from 50 or more speakers with different L1s who read 12 texts in their native language and 12 texts in English (only participants with upper intermediate level of English as an L2 were recruited). The MacMaster team collected data from the control group with L1 English. Thus, this is not only the largest cross-linguistic eye-tracking project, but also a project focusing on texts rather than individual sentences, which is not so common for eye-tracking corpora.
In addition to reading texts, participants passed many tests assessing the above-mentioned skills in L1 and/or in L2 (as well as the test on non-verbal intelligence that does not involve any language). For English, all these tests were readily available, but additional work was required for Russian (see project 6). After the two papers describing the general outcome of the project came out, we started working on a more detailed analysis of Russian speakers' data.
The MECO project has a spin-off — the ENglish Reading Online (ENRO) project. It uses the same materials as MECO, but the data are collecting online. This does not affect the tests, but self-paced reading instead of eye-tracking is used to study reading. Obviously, this reduces the ecological validity, but allows increasing the number of participants dramatically, and the collected data let us make many interesting comparisons.
Local collaborators: Svetlana Alexeeva, Daria Chernova, Anastasia Ivanenko. Students: Elizaveta Kuzmina, Marina Norkina.
Siegelman, N., Elgort, I., Brysbaert, M., Agrawal, N., Amenta, S., Arsenijević-Mijalković, J., Chang, C.S., Chernova, D., Chetail, F., Clarke, A.J.B., Content, A., Crepaldi, D., Davaabold, N., Delgersuren, S., Deutsch, A., Dibrova, V., Drieghe, D., Filipović-Đurđević, D., Finch, B., Frost, R., Gattei, C.A., Geva, E., Godfroid, A., Griener, L., Hernández-Rivera, E., Ivanenko, A., Järvikivi, L., Kawaletz, L., Khare, A., Lee, J.R., Lee, E.E., Manouilidou, C., Marelli, M., Mashanlo, T., Mišić, K., Miwa, K., Palma, P., Plag, I., Rezanova, Z., Riimed, E., Rueckl, J., Schroeder, S., Sekerina, I.A., Shalom, D.E., Slioussar, N., Slosar, N.M., Taler, V., Thériaul, K., Titone, D., Tumee, O., van de Wetering, R., Verma, A., Weiss, A.F., Wu, D.H., & Kuperman, V. (2024). Re-thinking L1-L2 similarities and differences in English proficiency: Insights from the ENglish Reading. Language Learning, 74, 249-294. Online first in 2023. Doi: 10.1111/lang.12586. Download.
Kuperman, V., Siegelman, N., Schroeder, S., Acartürk, C., Alexeeva, S., Amenta, S., Ahn, H.-D., Bertram, R., Bonandrini, R., Brysbaert, M., Chernova, D., Da Fonseca, S. M., Dirix, N., Duyck, W., Fella, A., Frost, R., Gattei, C. A., Kalaitzi, A., Kwon, N., Marelli, M., Papadopoulos, T. C., Protopapas, A., Savo, S., Shalom, D. E., Slioussar, N., Stein, R., Sui, L., Taboh, A., Tønnesen, V., & Usal, K.A. (2023). Text reading in English as a second language: Evidence from the Multilingual Eye-Movements Corpus (MECO). Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 45, 3–37. Online first in 2022. Doi: 10.1017/S0272263121000954. Download.
Siegelman, N., Schroeder, S., Acartürk, C., Alexeeva, S., Amenta, S., Ahn, H.-D., Bertram, R., Bonandrini, R., Brysbaert, M., Chernova, D., Da Fonseca, S. M., Dirix, N., Duyck, W., Fella, A., Frost, R., Gattei, C. A., Kalaitzi, A., Kwon, N., Marelli, M., Papadopoulos, T. C., Protopapas, A., Savo, S., Shalom, D. E., Slioussar, N., Stein, R., Sui, L., Taboh, A., Tønnesen, V., Usal, K.A., & Kuperman, V. (2022). Expanding horizons of cross-linguistic research on reading: The Multilingual Eye-movement Corpus (MECO). Behavior Research Methods, 54, 2843–2863. Doi: 10.3758/s13428-021-01772-6. Download.
6. Creating linguistic tests
After we got involved in the MECO project (see project 5), we got interested in developing tests to assess different linguistic competences in Russian. We created a test to assess orthographic skills and a test to assess the ability to parse complex syntactic structures (the SCT, or Sentence Comprehension Test, for Russian). I also took part in developing a Russian version of the Author Recognition Test to assess print exposure. The paper describing the SCT is already out, otherwise we are in the process of publishing the results.
Collaborators: Daria Chernova, Svetlana Alexeeva. Students: Polina Bakhturina, Artem Novozhilov.
Chernova, D., Novozhilov, A., & Slioussar, N. (2023). Sentence comprehension test for Russian: A tool to assess syntactic competence. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1035961. Doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1035961. Download.
7. Orthographic neighbors, orthographic errors and early stages of visual word recognition
A skilled reader does not read letter-by-letter — on an intuitive level, the fact that we often mix orthographically similar words or do not notice typos can serve as evidence. Approaching this question from a scientific perspective, linguists also rely on experiments with orthographic neighbors — words and pseudowords with similar spelling. We were the first to conduct such experiments on Russian, focusing on the feature that distinguishes it from previously studied languages: its rich morphology (it is interesting to find out how the process of letter identification and subsequent lexical access interacts with morphological analysis).
Another project explores orthographic errors. It was recently demonstrated that the overall frequency of spelling errors in a given word influences its processing (even when it is spelled correctly and the reader knows this): the first experiments were conducted on English, and we also demonstrated this for Russian. We also discovered that different types of orthographic errors behave differently in this respect. While working on this project, we created a test to measure spelling skills (see project 6).
Finally, in a small project we studied parafoveal processing (the role of word length and neighbourhood effects).
Collaborators: Svetlana Alexeeva, Daria Chernova. Students: Anastasia Petrova, Ekaterina Rudova, Taisia Metelkina, Nazar Krotov, Anastasia Udodenko, Ivan Gurkov.
Slioussar, N., Gurkov, I., & Chernova, D. (2022). Odni oshibki vrednee drugix: rol' tipa i chastotnosti orfograficheskix oshibok v obrabotke slov (in Russian, ‘Some errors are more harmful than others: the role of type and frequency of orthographic errors in word processing’). Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 21 (2), 1149-1157. Doi: 10.28995/2075-7182-2022-21-1149-1157. Download.
Chernova, D., Alexeeva, S., & Slioussar, N. (2020). Chemu nas uchat oshibki: trudnosti pri obrabotke slov s chastotnymi orfograficheskimi oshibkami (in Russian, ‘What do we learn from mistakes: processing difficulties with frequently misspelled words’). Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 19, 132-144. Download.
Slioussar, N., & Alexeeva, S. (2017). Orfograficheskie sosedi s zamenoj bukvy pri izuchenii mexanizmov leksicheskogo dostupa (in Russian: “Substitution orthographic neighbors in the study of lexical access”). Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 16, 407-418. Download.
Alexeeva, S., & Slioussar, N. (2017). Effekt dliny pri parafoveal'noj obrabotke slov vo vremja ДЌtenija (in Russian, ‘Parafoveal processing in reading: the role of word length’). Tomsk State University Journal: Philology, 45, 5-29. Doi: 10.17223/19986645/45/1. Download.
8. Consonant mutations and language change
Some Russian words have stem-final consonant mutations: e.g. ruka ‘hand’ – ruchka ‘handle, small hand’, nosit’ ‘to wear, to carry’ – noshu ‘wear1SG’. Using experimental studies and corpus research, we showed that these mutations are slowly decaying. Russian speakers often produce forms like molodee ‘younger’ instead of molozhѕe and experience even more difficulties with recent borrowings that are not part of literary language like frendit’ ‘to add to one’s friend list’, fotoshopit’ ‘to photoshop’ etc. Studying how different factors (phonological, morphological and semantic properties of words, token and type frequency, productivity, the literary norm) affect this process gives us new information on representing such information in the mental lexicon and grammar and on the mechanisms of language change. We also compared Russian to Ukrainian and Belarusian in which the situation with mutations is different in some respects.
Collaborators: Varvara Magomedova, Maria Kholodilova. Students: Anna Smetina, Vera Smirnova.
Magomedova, V., Smirova, V., & Slioussar, N. (2024). Neither dead nor alive: stem-final consonant mutations in Russian. Submitted ms. Download.
Magomedova, V., & Slioussar, N. (2017). Stem-final consonant mutations in modern Russian. In: Y. Oseki et al. (eds.). Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics: The New York Meeting 2015 (pp. 239-259). Ann Arbor, MI: Michigan Slavic Publications. Download.
Magomedova, V., & Slioussar, N. (2017). Paradigm leveling: The decay of consonant alternations in Russian. In: F. Kiefer et al. (eds.), Perspectives on Morphological Organization: Data and Analyses (pp. 123-137). Leiden: Brill. Doi 10.1163/9789004342934_00. Download.
Magomedova, V., & Slioussar, N. (2014). Dannye interneta v issledovanii jazykovyx izmenenij: analiz cheredovanij v russkix komparativax i programma dlja raboty s takimi dannymi (in Russian, ‘Internet data in the study of language change: A case study of alternations in Russian comparatives and a program to work with such data’). Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 13, 379-390. Download.
Slioussar, N., & Kholodilova, M. (2013). Paradigm leveling in non-standard Russian. In: A. Podobryaev et al. (eds.). Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics: The Second MIT Meeting 2011 (pp. 243-258). Ann Arbor, MI: Michigan University Press. Download.
9. Linguistic databases
Selecting stimuli with particular properties is a crucial stage of experimental research, and it is often hard or downright impossible to do without a searchable lexical database. We created StimulStat — the first lexical database for Russian in the form of a web application (http://stimul.cognitivestudies.ru). The database contains more than 50,000 most frequent Russian words (1.7 million word forms). These words and forms are characterized according to more than 70 properties, including frequency, length, phonological and grammatical properties, orthographic neighbourhood frequency and size, homonymy and polysemy. Some properties were retrieved from various dictionaries and presented collectively in a searchable form for the first time, the others were computed specifically for the database. In the paper describing the database we also present some interesting crosslinguistic differences that can be identified using such databases.
Small databases of Russian nouns and verbs. Another database we created contains information on frequency of various grammatical features of Russian nouns, based on the disambiguated subcorpus of the Russian National Corpus (RNC). One of the goals was to determine how frequent forms of nouns in different genders, cases and numbers, animate and inanimate nouns are, and how these characteristics depend on the inflectional paradigms (on the inflectional class and the stem type of the noun). The second goal was to determine the frequency of forms with different endings (depending on case, number, gender and inflectional class or not taking them into account). The database is available here and here. In addition to that, during my BA/MA studies I created a database of Russian verbs.
I also took part in the creation of the Multilingual Picture Database (MultiPic). It contains naming norms and familiarity scores for 500 coloured pictures in 32 languages.
Local collaborators: Svetlana Alexeeva, Daria Chernova. Students: Maria Samojlova, Pavel Prokopiev, Vladislav Meletyagin, Nikita Narchuk, Angelina Voronenko.
Duñabeitia, J. A., Baciero, A., Antoniou, K., Antoniou, M., Ataman, E., Baus, C., Ben-Shachar, M., Çağlar, O. C., Chromý, J., Comesaña, M., Filip, M., Filipović Đurđević, D., Gillon Dowens, M., Hatzidaki, A., Januška, J., Jusoh, Z., Kanj, R., Kim, S. Y., Kırkıcı, B., Leminen, A., Lohndal, T., Yap, N. T., Renvall, H., Rothman, J., Royle, P., Santesteban, M., Sevilla, Y., Slioussar, N., Vaughan-Evans, A., Wodniecka, Z., Wulff, S., & Pliatsikas, C. (2022). The Multilingual Picture Database. Scientific Data, 9, 431. Doi: 10.1038/s41597-022-01552-7. Download.
Alexeeva, S., Slioussar, N., & Chernova, D. (2018). StimulStat: a lexical database for Russian. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 2305-2315. Online first in 2017. Doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0994-3. Download.
Slioussar, N., & Samojlova M. (2015). Chastotnosti razlichnyx grammaticheskix xarakteristik i okonchanij u suschestvitel’nyx russkogo jazyka (in Russian, ‘Frequencies of different grammatical features and inflectional affixes in Russian nouns’). Proceedings of the conference ‘Dialogue 20’, http://www.dialog-21.ru/digests/dialog2015/materials/pdf/SlioussarNASamoilovaMV.pdf.
10. Some smaller projects (or the projects where my contribution was smaller)
The syntax of second-position clitics in Russian. Old Russian used to have many second-position, or Wackernagel, clitics, but then most of them were lost, and a couple of remaining ones changed their syntactic properties. We studied different factors that affect this change primarily on the example of the particle zhe, also focusing on a wider problem: how we can fruitfully combine data from corpus-based and experimental research (when these two sources give the same results and when they do not, and why).
Collaborators: Evdokia Valova. Students: Daria Tamilina.
Valova, E., & Slioussar, N. (2017). Issledovanie sintaksiДЌeskix svojstv enklitiki ‘Еѕe’: korpusnyj i eksperimental'nyj podhod (in Russian, ‘Syntactic properties of the Russian enclitic ‘zhe’: a corpus-based and an experimental approach’). Voprosy Jazykoznanija, 2, 33-48. Download.
Valova, E., & Slioussar, N. (2016). Sravnenie korpusnogo i eksperimental'nogo metoda na primere issledovanija sintaksiДЌeskix svojstv enklitiki ‘Еѕe’ (in Russian, ‘Comparing corpus-based and experimental research methods: a study of the syntactic properties of the Russian enclitic ‘zhe’’). Computational Linguistics and Intellectual Technologies, 15, 792-802. Download.
Control violation in converbial clauses in Russian. Errors like Proezzhѕaja pod mostom, u menja sletela shljapa ‘Passing under the bridge, my hat flew off’ are familiar to every speaker of Russian — we are all instructed not to make them and still do so once in a while. In this project, we study semantic and syntactic factors that influence the incidence of these errors in different corpora and their processing in the experimental settings, drawing conclusions for the syntactic theory of control.
Collaborators: Natalia Zevakhina, Svetlana Puzhaeva-Zhukova. Students: Evgeny Glazunov.
Zhukova, S., Zevakhina, N., Slioussar, N., & Glazunov, E. (2020). Non-canonical control in Russian converbial clauses. Russian Linguistics, 44, 129-143. Doi: 10.1007/s11185-020-09229-8. Download.
The syntax of prepositional phrases in Russian. When we say zaxodi ko mne na rabotu, this literally translates as ‘come to me to office’, but means ‘come to my office’. This small project focused on syntactic and semantic mechanisms allowing locative prepositional phrases to develop this possessive meaning in Russian.
Collaborators: Ora Matushansky, Nora Boneh, Lea Nash.
Matushansky, O., Boneh, N., Nash, L., & Slioussar, N. (2020). To PPs in their proper place. In: T. Ionin & J.E. MacDonald (eds.). Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics: The Urbana-Champaign Meeting 2017 (pp. 228-245). Ann Arbor, MI: Michigan Slavic Publications. Download.
The Possible Word Constraint, or PWC. The PWC is a principle regulating segmentation of continuous speech, according to which word boundaries should not be postulated if a remaining segment contains only consonants because this is not a feasible word. The PWC was initially formulated for English and claimed to hold universally after being confirmed for various other languages. Languages allowing for words without vowels present a challenge to the PWC, but, before we started our project, only two studies were conducted and their results did not converge. Our experiments on Russian, a language that has several single-consonant words (prepositions and particles), addressed several problems from the previous studies and eventually demonstrated that the PWC does not operate in Russian, undermining the claim about its universality.
Collaborators: Svetlana Alexeeva. Students: Anastasia Frolova.
Alexeeva, S., Frolova, A., & Slioussar, N. (2017). Data from Russian help to determine in which languages the Possible Word Constraint applies. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46, 629-640. Online first in 2016. Doi 10.1007/s10936-016-9458-7. Download.
Derivational morphology in the mental lexicon. Russian is rich in derivational morphology: it is possible to form many new words using various prefixes and suffixes. In this project, we ask how derived words are connected to their base word in the mental lexicon, whether and how they influence lexical access to it, and which connections are closer: formed by suffixation or by prefixation. The work on this project was continued by Anastasia Chuprina in her PhD thesis.
Collaborators: Anastasia Chuprina.
Slioussar, N., & Chuprina, A. (2016). How derivational links affect lexical access: evidence from Russian verbs and nouns. Italian Journal of Linguistics, 28, 115-136. Download.
Processing non-selected meanings of ambiguous stimuli. In this fMRI study, we looked at brain systems dealing with multiple meanings of ambiguous stimuli. We focused on the processing of non-selected meanings: are they actively suppressed or not? Our study using a modified version of the word fragment completion task suggests a positive answer.
Collaborators: Maxim Kireev and his colleagues from the N.P. Bekhtereva Institute of the Human Brain, Russian Academy of Sciences, Valeria Gershkovich, Nadezhda Moroshkina, Viktor Allakhverdov and Tatiana Chernigovskaya.
Kireev, M., Korotkov, A., Masharipov, R., Zheltyakova, M., Cherednichenko, D., Gershkovich, V., Moroshkina, N., Slioussar, N., Allakhverdov, V., & Chernigovskaya, T. (2022). Suppression of non‑selected solutions as a possible brain mechanism for ambiguity resolution in the word fragment task completion task. Scientific Reports, 12, 1829. Doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05646-5. Download.
Processing metaphors. Despite extensive research, what mechanisms are involved in understanding metaphors is still a matter of debate. In particular, it is unclear whether while processing metaphoric expressions, we also access their literal meaning and, if yes, whether it is easier or more difficult to process (either because it is accessed first, or because it is computed compositionally, while metaphoric expressions are stored and retrieved as a whole). Our experiments demonstrated that the answer depends on the type of metaphor (novel, conventional, idiomatic).
Collaborators: Natalia Cherepovskaia, Tatiana Petrova and other colleagues from the Laboratory for Cognitive Studies, Saint Petersburg State University.
Slioussar, N., Petrova, T., Mikhailovskaya, E., Cherepovskaia, N., Prokopenya, V., Chernova, D., & Chernigovskaya, T. (2017). Eksperimental'nye issledovanija grammatiki: slovosochetanija s bukval'nym i nebukval'nym znacheniem (in Russian, ‘Experimental studies of the grammar: expressions with literal and non-literal meaning’). Voprosy Jazykoznanija, 3, 83-98. Download.
Cherepovskaia, N., & Slioussar, N. (2014). Comprehending metaphors of different types: Evidence from Russian. In: V. Torrens & L. Escobar (eds.). Papers on the processing of lexicon and syntax (pp. 25-42). Cambridge: Cambridge Scholars Publishing. Download.